Fluctuating mast production does not drive Scandinavian brown bear behavior

ABSTRACT Bears often rely on soft or hard mast during fall hyperphagia when they increase body mass in preparation for winter hibernation. Studies of North American and Japanese bear populations suggest they respond to years of mast crop failure by increasing movement rates and roaming farther, with...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:The Journal of Wildlife Management
Main Authors: Hertel, Anne G., Zedrosser, Andreas, Kindberg, Jonas, Langvall, Ola, Swenson, Jon E.
Other Authors: Austrian Science Fund
Format: Article in Journal/Newspaper
Language:English
Published: Wiley 2018
Subjects:
Online Access:http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/jwmg.21619
https://api.wiley.com/onlinelibrary/tdm/v1/articles/10.1002%2Fjwmg.21619
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1002/jwmg.21619
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full-xml/10.1002/jwmg.21619
Description
Summary:ABSTRACT Bears often rely on soft or hard mast during fall hyperphagia when they increase body mass in preparation for winter hibernation. Studies of North American and Japanese bear populations suggest they respond to years of mast crop failure by increasing movement rates and roaming farther, with an increase in human–wildlife conflicts. In southcentral Sweden, brown bears ( Ursus arctos ) primarily feed on bilberry ( Vaccinium myrtillus ) and lingonberry ( V. vitis‐idaea ) during hyperphagia. We hypothesized that berry production affects movement, activity, and space use behaviors of bears in Sweden, which have the potential to increase human–bear encounters. We tested whether seasonal activity patterns, human settlement visits, and clearcut selection ratios were affected by bilberry and lingonberry productivity between 2007 and 2017 with linear and generalized linear mixed effect models. Contrary to our expectations, we did not find that bears moved more or maintained larger home ranges in years of low berry production. Bears were slightly more active in years of higher bilberry production, but variation in behavior was primarily explained by demographic group and individual differences. Bears rarely visited human settlements and the number of visits did not increase in relation to shortage of natural foods. Likewise, population‐level selection for clearcuts was unrelated to berry production but reflected a differential food search behavior in the 2 peak berry seasons, with higher clearcut selection ratios during the lingonberry season. Only 12 bears regularly used agricultural fields, which were too few to relate field visits to berry production, but all bears visited fields more often during the later lingonberry season. We suggest that weaker fluctuations in berry production, a continuous spatial distribution of berries, and an apparent absence of forage‐limiting exploitative intra‐ or interspecific competition contribute to brown bears in Scandinavia being less food limited than bears in North America or ...