Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises
Toothed whales use sonar to detect, locate, and track prey. They adjust emitted sound intensity, auditory sensitivity and click rate to target range, and terminate prey pursuits with high-repetition-rate, low-intensity buzzes. However, their narrow acoustic field of view (FOV) is considered stable t...
id |
fttriple:oai:gotriple.eu:50|dedup_wf_001::6f1f587eea9a52bcb245963ef99ef10e |
---|---|
record_format |
openpolar |
institution |
Open Polar |
collection |
Unknown |
op_collection_id |
fttriple |
language |
English |
topic |
Beam Biosonar Buzz Convergent evolution Directionality Prey capture Adaptation Physiological Phocoena/anatomy & histology Predatory Behavior/physiology Male Echolocation/physiology Sound Biological Evolution Magnetic Resonance Imaging Animals Chiroptera/anatomy & histology Animal Structures/anatomy & histology Video Recording Female Vocalization Animal/physiology Feeding Behavior/physiology Research Article Ecology Neuroscience phocoena phocoena beam directionality other QH301 Biology QL Zoology DAS QH301 QL General Biochemistry Genetics and Molecular Biology General Immunology and Microbiology General Neuroscience General Medicine geo envir |
spellingShingle |
Beam Biosonar Buzz Convergent evolution Directionality Prey capture Adaptation Physiological Phocoena/anatomy & histology Predatory Behavior/physiology Male Echolocation/physiology Sound Biological Evolution Magnetic Resonance Imaging Animals Chiroptera/anatomy & histology Animal Structures/anatomy & histology Video Recording Female Vocalization Animal/physiology Feeding Behavior/physiology Research Article Ecology Neuroscience phocoena phocoena beam directionality other QH301 Biology QL Zoology DAS QH301 QL General Biochemistry Genetics and Molecular Biology General Immunology and Microbiology General Neuroscience General Medicine geo envir John M. Ratcliffe Peter T. Madsen Mark P. Johnson Christian Bech Christensen Jens C. Koblitz Danuta M. Wisniewska Magnus Wahlberg Kristian Beedholm Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
topic_facet |
Beam Biosonar Buzz Convergent evolution Directionality Prey capture Adaptation Physiological Phocoena/anatomy & histology Predatory Behavior/physiology Male Echolocation/physiology Sound Biological Evolution Magnetic Resonance Imaging Animals Chiroptera/anatomy & histology Animal Structures/anatomy & histology Video Recording Female Vocalization Animal/physiology Feeding Behavior/physiology Research Article Ecology Neuroscience phocoena phocoena beam directionality other QH301 Biology QL Zoology DAS QH301 QL General Biochemistry Genetics and Molecular Biology General Immunology and Microbiology General Neuroscience General Medicine geo envir |
description |
Toothed whales use sonar to detect, locate, and track prey. They adjust emitted sound intensity, auditory sensitivity and click rate to target range, and terminate prey pursuits with high-repetition-rate, low-intensity buzzes. However, their narrow acoustic field of view (FOV) is considered stable throughout target approach, which could facilitate prey escape at close-range. Here, we show that, like some bats, harbour porpoises can broaden their biosonar beam during the terminal phase of attack but, unlike bats, maintain the ability to change beamwidth within this phase. Based on video, MRI, and acoustic-tag recordings, we propose this flexibility is modulated by the melon and implemented to accommodate dynamic spatial relationships with prey and acoustic complexity of surroundings. Despite independent evolution and different means of sound generation and transmission, whales and bats adaptively change their FOV, suggesting that beamwidth flexibility has been an important driver in the evolution of echolocation for prey tracking. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05651.001 eLife digest Bats and toothed whales such as porpoises have independently evolved the same solution for hunting prey when it is hard to see. Bats hunt in the dark with little light to allow them to see the insects they chase. Porpoises hunt in murky water where different ocean environments can quickly obscure fish from view. So, both bats and porpoises evolved to emit a beam of sound and then track their prey based on the echoes of that sound bouncing off the prey and other objects. This process is called echolocation. A narrow beam of sound can help a porpoise or bat track distant prey. But as either animal closes in on its prey such a narrow sound beam can be a disadvantage because prey can easily escape to one side. Scientists recently found that bats can widen their sound beam as they close in on prey by changing the frequency—or pitch—of the signal they emit or by adjusting how they open their mouth. Porpoises, by contrast, create ... |
author2 |
University of St Andrews.School of Biology University of St Andrews.Marine Alliance for Science & Technology Scotland University of St Andrews.Sea Mammal Research Unit University of St Andrews.Sound Tags Group University of St Andrews.Bioacoustics group University of St Andrews.Scottish Oceans Institute |
format |
Article in Journal/Newspaper |
author |
John M. Ratcliffe Peter T. Madsen Mark P. Johnson Christian Bech Christensen Jens C. Koblitz Danuta M. Wisniewska Magnus Wahlberg Kristian Beedholm |
author_facet |
John M. Ratcliffe Peter T. Madsen Mark P. Johnson Christian Bech Christensen Jens C. Koblitz Danuta M. Wisniewska Magnus Wahlberg Kristian Beedholm |
author_sort |
John M. Ratcliffe |
title |
Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
title_short |
Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
title_full |
Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
title_fullStr |
Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
title_full_unstemmed |
Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
title_sort |
flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises |
publishDate |
2015 |
url |
https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.pdf http://www.marinebioacoustics.com/files/2015/Wisniewska_etal_2015.pdf http://www.batsandmoths.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/e05651.full_.pdf https://portal.findresearcher.sdu.dk/da/publications/e7f9c634-b13c-477c-9ab0-19847b725273 https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4413254 https://pure.au.dk/portal/da/publications/rangedependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises-phocoena-phocoena(fc3b98c6-cabf-49fc-a07f-4908791e5ea6).html http://elifesciences.org/lookup/doi/10.7554/eLife.05651 https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.xml https://elifesciences.org/articles/05651 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4413254 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/handle/10023/6646 https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_3192952 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/bitstream/handle/10023/6646/wisniewska2015elifee05651.pdf;sequence=1 https://paperity.org/p/73665855/range-dependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/handle/123456789/50989 https://publikationen.uni-tuebingen.de/xmlui/handle/10900/64955 https://academic.microsoft.com/#/detail/2132396513 |
genre |
Phocoena phocoena toothed whales |
genre_facet |
Phocoena phocoena toothed whales |
op_source |
oai:sdu.dk:publications/e7f9c634-b13c-477c-9ab0-19847b725273 oai:dnet:od_______908::34a5c06ce155494793b1400540b15684 10.7554/elife.05651 25922991 oai:pubmedcentral.nih.gov:4413254 oai:kops.uni-konstanz.de:123456789/50989 oai:pure.atira.dk:publications/fc3b98c6-cabf-49fc-a07f-4908791e5ea6 oai:research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk:10023/6646 2132396513 10|opendoar____::1aa057313c28fa4a40c5bc084b11d276 10|infrastruct_::f66f1bd369679b5b077dcdf006089556 openaire____::1256f046-bf1f-4afc-8b47-d0b147148b18 10|opendoar____::eda80a3d5b344bc40f3bc04f65b7a357 10|opendoar____::8b6dd7db9af49e67306feb59a8bdc52c 10|opendoar____::8df707a948fac1b4a0f97aa554886ec8 10|openaire____::d76e4d42b3bd658259e8bf9c37ef448f 10|openaire____::9e3be59865b2c1c335d32dae2fe7b254 10|openaire____::55045bd2a65019fd8e6741a755395c8c 10|opendoar____::892c91e0a653ba19df81a90f89d99bcd 10|openaire____::081b82f96300b6a6e3d282bad31cb6e2 10|doajarticles::918ee31b7bf0a155228fcc9e17783d80 10|openaire____::8ac8380272269217cb09a928c8caa993 10|openaire____::5f532a3fc4f1ea403f37070f59a7a53a 10|openaire____::806360c771262b4d6770e7cdf04b5c5a |
op_relation |
https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.pdf http://www.marinebioacoustics.com/files/2015/Wisniewska_etal_2015.pdf http://www.batsandmoths.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/e05651.full_.pdf https://portal.findresearcher.sdu.dk/da/publications/e7f9c634-b13c-477c-9ab0-19847b725273 https://dx.doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4413254 https://pure.au.dk/portal/da/publications/rangedependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises-phocoena-phocoena(fc3b98c6-cabf-49fc-a07f-4908791e5ea6).html http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 http://elifesciences.org/lookup/doi/10.7554/eLife.05651 https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.xml https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 https://elifesciences.org/articles/05651 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4413254 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/handle/10023/6646 https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_3192952 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/bitstream/handle/10023/6646/wisniewska2015elifee05651.pdf;sequence=1 https://paperity.org/p/73665855/range-dependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/handle/123456789/50989 https://publikationen.uni-tuebingen.de/xmlui/handle/10900/64955 https://academic.microsoft.com/#/detail/2132396513 |
op_rights |
lic_creative-commons |
op_doi |
https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05651 |
container_title |
eLife |
container_volume |
4 |
_version_ |
1766168038899449856 |
spelling |
fttriple:oai:gotriple.eu:50|dedup_wf_001::6f1f587eea9a52bcb245963ef99ef10e 2023-05-15T17:59:15+02:00 Flexible field of view in echolocating porpoises John M. Ratcliffe Peter T. Madsen Mark P. Johnson Christian Bech Christensen Jens C. Koblitz Danuta M. Wisniewska Magnus Wahlberg Kristian Beedholm University of St Andrews.School of Biology University of St Andrews.Marine Alliance for Science & Technology Scotland University of St Andrews.Sea Mammal Research Unit University of St Andrews.Sound Tags Group University of St Andrews.Bioacoustics group University of St Andrews.Scottish Oceans Institute 2015-03-20 https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.pdf http://www.marinebioacoustics.com/files/2015/Wisniewska_etal_2015.pdf http://www.batsandmoths.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/e05651.full_.pdf https://portal.findresearcher.sdu.dk/da/publications/e7f9c634-b13c-477c-9ab0-19847b725273 https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4413254 https://pure.au.dk/portal/da/publications/rangedependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises-phocoena-phocoena(fc3b98c6-cabf-49fc-a07f-4908791e5ea6).html http://elifesciences.org/lookup/doi/10.7554/eLife.05651 https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.xml https://elifesciences.org/articles/05651 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4413254 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/handle/10023/6646 https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_3192952 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/bitstream/handle/10023/6646/wisniewska2015elifee05651.pdf;sequence=1 https://paperity.org/p/73665855/range-dependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/handle/123456789/50989 https://publikationen.uni-tuebingen.de/xmlui/handle/10900/64955 https://academic.microsoft.com/#/detail/2132396513 en eng https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.pdf http://www.marinebioacoustics.com/files/2015/Wisniewska_etal_2015.pdf http://www.batsandmoths.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/04/e05651.full_.pdf https://portal.findresearcher.sdu.dk/da/publications/e7f9c634-b13c-477c-9ab0-19847b725273 https://dx.doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC4413254 https://pure.au.dk/portal/da/publications/rangedependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises-phocoena-phocoena(fc3b98c6-cabf-49fc-a07f-4908791e5ea6).html http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 http://elifesciences.org/lookup/doi/10.7554/eLife.05651 https://cdn.elifesciences.org/articles/05651/elife-05651-v2.xml https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 https://elifesciences.org/articles/05651 https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4413254 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/handle/10023/6646 https://pure.mpg.de/pubman/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_3192952 https://research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk/bitstream/handle/10023/6646/wisniewska2015elifee05651.pdf;sequence=1 https://paperity.org/p/73665855/range-dependent-flexibility-in-the-acoustic-field-of-view-of-echolocating-porpoises https://kops.uni-konstanz.de/handle/123456789/50989 https://publikationen.uni-tuebingen.de/xmlui/handle/10900/64955 https://academic.microsoft.com/#/detail/2132396513 lic_creative-commons oai:sdu.dk:publications/e7f9c634-b13c-477c-9ab0-19847b725273 oai:dnet:od_______908::34a5c06ce155494793b1400540b15684 10.7554/elife.05651 25922991 oai:pubmedcentral.nih.gov:4413254 oai:kops.uni-konstanz.de:123456789/50989 oai:pure.atira.dk:publications/fc3b98c6-cabf-49fc-a07f-4908791e5ea6 oai:research-repository.st-andrews.ac.uk:10023/6646 2132396513 10|opendoar____::1aa057313c28fa4a40c5bc084b11d276 10|infrastruct_::f66f1bd369679b5b077dcdf006089556 openaire____::1256f046-bf1f-4afc-8b47-d0b147148b18 10|opendoar____::eda80a3d5b344bc40f3bc04f65b7a357 10|opendoar____::8b6dd7db9af49e67306feb59a8bdc52c 10|opendoar____::8df707a948fac1b4a0f97aa554886ec8 10|openaire____::d76e4d42b3bd658259e8bf9c37ef448f 10|openaire____::9e3be59865b2c1c335d32dae2fe7b254 10|openaire____::55045bd2a65019fd8e6741a755395c8c 10|opendoar____::892c91e0a653ba19df81a90f89d99bcd 10|openaire____::081b82f96300b6a6e3d282bad31cb6e2 10|doajarticles::918ee31b7bf0a155228fcc9e17783d80 10|openaire____::8ac8380272269217cb09a928c8caa993 10|openaire____::5f532a3fc4f1ea403f37070f59a7a53a 10|openaire____::806360c771262b4d6770e7cdf04b5c5a Beam Biosonar Buzz Convergent evolution Directionality Prey capture Adaptation Physiological Phocoena/anatomy & histology Predatory Behavior/physiology Male Echolocation/physiology Sound Biological Evolution Magnetic Resonance Imaging Animals Chiroptera/anatomy & histology Animal Structures/anatomy & histology Video Recording Female Vocalization Animal/physiology Feeding Behavior/physiology Research Article Ecology Neuroscience phocoena phocoena beam directionality other QH301 Biology QL Zoology DAS QH301 QL General Biochemistry Genetics and Molecular Biology General Immunology and Microbiology General Neuroscience General Medicine geo envir Journal Article https://vocabularies.coar-repositories.org/resource_types/c_6501/ 2015 fttriple https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.05651 https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05651 2023-01-22T17:22:20Z Toothed whales use sonar to detect, locate, and track prey. They adjust emitted sound intensity, auditory sensitivity and click rate to target range, and terminate prey pursuits with high-repetition-rate, low-intensity buzzes. However, their narrow acoustic field of view (FOV) is considered stable throughout target approach, which could facilitate prey escape at close-range. Here, we show that, like some bats, harbour porpoises can broaden their biosonar beam during the terminal phase of attack but, unlike bats, maintain the ability to change beamwidth within this phase. Based on video, MRI, and acoustic-tag recordings, we propose this flexibility is modulated by the melon and implemented to accommodate dynamic spatial relationships with prey and acoustic complexity of surroundings. Despite independent evolution and different means of sound generation and transmission, whales and bats adaptively change their FOV, suggesting that beamwidth flexibility has been an important driver in the evolution of echolocation for prey tracking. DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.7554/eLife.05651.001 eLife digest Bats and toothed whales such as porpoises have independently evolved the same solution for hunting prey when it is hard to see. Bats hunt in the dark with little light to allow them to see the insects they chase. Porpoises hunt in murky water where different ocean environments can quickly obscure fish from view. So, both bats and porpoises evolved to emit a beam of sound and then track their prey based on the echoes of that sound bouncing off the prey and other objects. This process is called echolocation. A narrow beam of sound can help a porpoise or bat track distant prey. But as either animal closes in on its prey such a narrow sound beam can be a disadvantage because prey can easily escape to one side. Scientists recently found that bats can widen their sound beam as they close in on prey by changing the frequency—or pitch—of the signal they emit or by adjusting how they open their mouth. Porpoises, by contrast, create ... Article in Journal/Newspaper Phocoena phocoena toothed whales Unknown eLife 4 |