Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace

The objective of this study was to examine workers' mental interpretation models developed in response to occupational chemical exposure. The study was performed in six companies within the reinforced plastics industry in northern Sweden, in which styrene was used; 32 workers participated in th...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:Risk Analysis
Main Authors: Anita Elisabeth Pettersson†Strömbäck, Ingrid Elisabeth Liljelind, Steven Nordin, Bengt Järvholm
Format: Article in Journal/Newspaper
Language:unknown
Subjects:
Online Access:https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x
id ftrepec:oai:RePEc:wly:riskan:v:30:y:2010:i:3:p:488-500
record_format openpolar
spelling ftrepec:oai:RePEc:wly:riskan:v:30:y:2010:i:3:p:488-500 2023-05-15T17:44:48+02:00 Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace Anita Elisabeth Pettersson†Strömbäck Ingrid Elisabeth Liljelind Steven Nordin Bengt Järvholm https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x unknown https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x article ftrepec https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x 2020-12-04T13:32:28Z The objective of this study was to examine workers' mental interpretation models developed in response to occupational chemical exposure. The study was performed in six companies within the reinforced plastics industry in northern Sweden, in which styrene was used; 32 workers participated in the study. Each worker performed between four and seven exposure measurements. Before receiving each result of the second to seventh measurements, the workers were asked to predict the level of their next exposure measurement. Their predictions were evaluated with respect to two judgmental principles: coherence (that the predictions are based on logical decision rules, that is, the mean value of the prior exposure levels); and correspondence (the predictions have high empirical accuracy) by calculating the mean absolute percent forcast error (MAPE). The coherence principle was tested by comparing each of the workers' predictions with the mean, median, and last exposure level (last value) of the prior measurements. The correspondence principle was tested by comparing the worker's prediction with the outcome of the measurement. The coherence principle was found to be the best descriptor of the workers' predictions and the median model had the best fit. The mean model had a similar but significantly poorer fit (MAPE values of 29 and 31, respectively). The correspondence model had a poor fit with a MAPE of 54. The workers' predictions were generally lower than their average exposures. We conclude that the workers' interpretation model can be best described by a coherence model rather than by a correspondence model. Article in Journal/Newspaper Northern Sweden RePEc (Research Papers in Economics) Risk Analysis 30 3 488 500
institution Open Polar
collection RePEc (Research Papers in Economics)
op_collection_id ftrepec
language unknown
description The objective of this study was to examine workers' mental interpretation models developed in response to occupational chemical exposure. The study was performed in six companies within the reinforced plastics industry in northern Sweden, in which styrene was used; 32 workers participated in the study. Each worker performed between four and seven exposure measurements. Before receiving each result of the second to seventh measurements, the workers were asked to predict the level of their next exposure measurement. Their predictions were evaluated with respect to two judgmental principles: coherence (that the predictions are based on logical decision rules, that is, the mean value of the prior exposure levels); and correspondence (the predictions have high empirical accuracy) by calculating the mean absolute percent forcast error (MAPE). The coherence principle was tested by comparing each of the workers' predictions with the mean, median, and last exposure level (last value) of the prior measurements. The correspondence principle was tested by comparing the worker's prediction with the outcome of the measurement. The coherence principle was found to be the best descriptor of the workers' predictions and the median model had the best fit. The mean model had a similar but significantly poorer fit (MAPE values of 29 and 31, respectively). The correspondence model had a poor fit with a MAPE of 54. The workers' predictions were generally lower than their average exposures. We conclude that the workers' interpretation model can be best described by a coherence model rather than by a correspondence model.
format Article in Journal/Newspaper
author Anita Elisabeth Pettersson†Strömbäck
Ingrid Elisabeth Liljelind
Steven Nordin
Bengt Järvholm
spellingShingle Anita Elisabeth Pettersson†Strömbäck
Ingrid Elisabeth Liljelind
Steven Nordin
Bengt Järvholm
Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace
author_facet Anita Elisabeth Pettersson†Strömbäck
Ingrid Elisabeth Liljelind
Steven Nordin
Bengt Järvholm
author_sort Anita Elisabeth Pettersson†Strömbäck
title Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace
title_short Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace
title_full Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace
title_fullStr Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace
title_full_unstemmed Workers' Mental Models of Chemical Exposure in the Workplace
title_sort workers' mental models of chemical exposure in the workplace
url https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x
genre Northern Sweden
genre_facet Northern Sweden
op_relation https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x
op_doi https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2009.01347.x
container_title Risk Analysis
container_volume 30
container_issue 3
container_start_page 488
op_container_end_page 500
_version_ 1766147097470435328