Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning

The presence of sex-stereotyped behavior in monomorphic animals, where there are no sexual differences in form to account for sexual differences in function, is often attributed to intraspecific competition or to differential parental investment. The possibility that the use of different behavioral...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Kyle Hamish Elliott, Anthony J. Gaston, Douglas Crump
Format: Article in Journal/Newspaper
Language:unknown
Subjects:
Online Access:http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arq076
id ftrepec:oai:RePEc:oup:beheco:v:21:y:2010:i:5:p:1024-1032
record_format openpolar
spelling ftrepec:oai:RePEc:oup:beheco:v:21:y:2010:i:5:p:1024-1032 2024-04-14T08:20:39+00:00 Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning Kyle Hamish Elliott Anthony J. Gaston Douglas Crump http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arq076 unknown http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arq076 article ftrepec 2024-03-19T10:27:59Z The presence of sex-stereotyped behavior in monomorphic animals, where there are no sexual differences in form to account for sexual differences in function, is often attributed to intraspecific competition or to differential parental investment. The possibility that the use of different behavioral strategies by each parent may increase reproductive success for both partners through risk partitioning is seldom considered. We studied thick-billed murres (Uria lomvia), where the male exclusively feeds the offspring during the late chick rearing. During the period of biparental care, males fed on "risk-averse" prey (consistent across time and space; unitized risk = 0.29), whereas females fed on "risk-prone" prey (risk = 0.59). Males fed at night at 1 colony, during the day at 2 colonies, and there was no pattern at another colony. We suggest that these differences reflect the availability of risk-prone prey. Modeling suggested that mixed-risk pairs had higher success than "risky" or "riskless" pairs. Males accumulated reserves and reduced chick provisioning just prior to fledging. Thus, sex-specific patterns at 1 period (male-only care during postfledging) may have led to sex-specific patterns at earlier periods through the need for specialization in foraging habits and risk. We propose that risk partitioning may contribute to the prevalence of sex-specific behaviors in monomorphic animals and that patterns are likely context specific rather than species specific. Copyright 2010, Oxford University Press. Article in Journal/Newspaper Uria lomvia uria RePEc (Research Papers in Economics)
institution Open Polar
collection RePEc (Research Papers in Economics)
op_collection_id ftrepec
language unknown
description The presence of sex-stereotyped behavior in monomorphic animals, where there are no sexual differences in form to account for sexual differences in function, is often attributed to intraspecific competition or to differential parental investment. The possibility that the use of different behavioral strategies by each parent may increase reproductive success for both partners through risk partitioning is seldom considered. We studied thick-billed murres (Uria lomvia), where the male exclusively feeds the offspring during the late chick rearing. During the period of biparental care, males fed on "risk-averse" prey (consistent across time and space; unitized risk = 0.29), whereas females fed on "risk-prone" prey (risk = 0.59). Males fed at night at 1 colony, during the day at 2 colonies, and there was no pattern at another colony. We suggest that these differences reflect the availability of risk-prone prey. Modeling suggested that mixed-risk pairs had higher success than "risky" or "riskless" pairs. Males accumulated reserves and reduced chick provisioning just prior to fledging. Thus, sex-specific patterns at 1 period (male-only care during postfledging) may have led to sex-specific patterns at earlier periods through the need for specialization in foraging habits and risk. We propose that risk partitioning may contribute to the prevalence of sex-specific behaviors in monomorphic animals and that patterns are likely context specific rather than species specific. Copyright 2010, Oxford University Press.
format Article in Journal/Newspaper
author Kyle Hamish Elliott
Anthony J. Gaston
Douglas Crump
spellingShingle Kyle Hamish Elliott
Anthony J. Gaston
Douglas Crump
Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
author_facet Kyle Hamish Elliott
Anthony J. Gaston
Douglas Crump
author_sort Kyle Hamish Elliott
title Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_short Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_full Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_fullStr Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_full_unstemmed Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_sort sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
url http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arq076
genre Uria lomvia
uria
genre_facet Uria lomvia
uria
op_relation http://hdl.handle.net/10.1093/beheco/arq076
_version_ 1796299016591376384