The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?

This paper investigates the production of the low front vowel /æ/ which undergoes “tensing” and “raising” (Ferguson, 1972; Labov, 1989) when followed by a nasal consonant in some dialects of English (Labov, Ash and Boberg 2006). Speakers in their early twenties, and life long residents of the provin...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Author: De Decker, Paul
Format: Conference Object
Language:English
Published: 2014
Subjects:
Online Access:https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/
https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/1/CLA_poster_2014.pdf
https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/3/CLA_poster_2014.pdf
id ftmemorialuniv:oai:research.library.mun.ca:6422
record_format openpolar
spelling ftmemorialuniv:oai:research.library.mun.ca:6422 2023-10-01T03:57:39+02:00 The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense? De Decker, Paul 2014-05-25 application/pdf https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/ https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/1/CLA_poster_2014.pdf https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/3/CLA_poster_2014.pdf en eng https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/1/CLA_poster_2014.pdf https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/3/CLA_poster_2014.pdf De Decker, Paul <https://research.library.mun.ca/view/creator_az/De_Decker=3APaul=3A=3A.html> (2014) The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense? In: Canadian Linguistics Association, May 24–26, 2014, St. Catharine's, Ontario. (Unpublished) Conference or Workshop Item NonPeerReviewed 2014 ftmemorialuniv 2023-09-03T06:45:50Z This paper investigates the production of the low front vowel /æ/ which undergoes “tensing” and “raising” (Ferguson, 1972; Labov, 1989) when followed by a nasal consonant in some dialects of English (Labov, Ash and Boberg 2006). Speakers in their early twenties, and life long residents of the province of Newfoundland read a word list which included five tokens of both hand and hat. All speakers exhibited significantly lower F1 and higher F2 values for the vowel in the word hand compared to hat. As a way to explain the acoustic differences found in nasal systems, we test the model outlined by Krakow et al. (1988), that such lowering and raising is a result of co-articulation between the vowel and the following nasal consonant. We predict that: 1. If nasal co-articulation is responsible for tensing, then nasality, measured by A1-P0 (Chen 1997, Chen et al. 2007) should be higher in the nasal environment compared oral ones (i.e. elsewhere). 2. (a) Nasality and (b) its concomitant effects on F1 and F2 should be weakest early in the vowel and strongest immediately preceding the nasal consonant. This follows from Cohn's (1993) formulation of co-articulatory nasalization as a phonetically gradient rule as opposed to a categorical phonological one. All acoustic analyses were conducted using Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2013) at two temporal locations (20% and 80%) into each vowel token. Two-tailed t-tests revealed significantly lower A1-P0 values (more nasality) in the nasal environment for Speaker 1 (t (18) = 5.5011, p < 0.0001), but not for Speaker 2 (t (18) = 1.9266, p = 0.07) or Speaker 3, (t (18) = 0.2687, p = 0.79). This presents a challenge to Hypothesis (1) above suggesting that tensing might not directly related to nasalization. To test Hypothesis (2), three two-tailed t-tests were run for each speaker to examine the effect of nasality over the course of /æ/. No statistically significant differences were found in A1-P0 across the duration of the vowel for Speaker 1 (t (8) =1.008, p = 0.34). However, significant ... Conference Object Newfoundland Memorial University of Newfoundland: Research Repository Ferguson ENVELOPE(-168.583,-168.583,-84.933,-84.933) Boberg ENVELOPE(22.034,22.034,69.974,69.974)
institution Open Polar
collection Memorial University of Newfoundland: Research Repository
op_collection_id ftmemorialuniv
language English
description This paper investigates the production of the low front vowel /æ/ which undergoes “tensing” and “raising” (Ferguson, 1972; Labov, 1989) when followed by a nasal consonant in some dialects of English (Labov, Ash and Boberg 2006). Speakers in their early twenties, and life long residents of the province of Newfoundland read a word list which included five tokens of both hand and hat. All speakers exhibited significantly lower F1 and higher F2 values for the vowel in the word hand compared to hat. As a way to explain the acoustic differences found in nasal systems, we test the model outlined by Krakow et al. (1988), that such lowering and raising is a result of co-articulation between the vowel and the following nasal consonant. We predict that: 1. If nasal co-articulation is responsible for tensing, then nasality, measured by A1-P0 (Chen 1997, Chen et al. 2007) should be higher in the nasal environment compared oral ones (i.e. elsewhere). 2. (a) Nasality and (b) its concomitant effects on F1 and F2 should be weakest early in the vowel and strongest immediately preceding the nasal consonant. This follows from Cohn's (1993) formulation of co-articulatory nasalization as a phonetically gradient rule as opposed to a categorical phonological one. All acoustic analyses were conducted using Praat (Boersma and Weenink 2013) at two temporal locations (20% and 80%) into each vowel token. Two-tailed t-tests revealed significantly lower A1-P0 values (more nasality) in the nasal environment for Speaker 1 (t (18) = 5.5011, p < 0.0001), but not for Speaker 2 (t (18) = 1.9266, p = 0.07) or Speaker 3, (t (18) = 0.2687, p = 0.79). This presents a challenge to Hypothesis (1) above suggesting that tensing might not directly related to nasalization. To test Hypothesis (2), three two-tailed t-tests were run for each speaker to examine the effect of nasality over the course of /æ/. No statistically significant differences were found in A1-P0 across the duration of the vowel for Speaker 1 (t (8) =1.008, p = 0.34). However, significant ...
format Conference Object
author De Decker, Paul
spellingShingle De Decker, Paul
The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?
author_facet De Decker, Paul
author_sort De Decker, Paul
title The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?
title_short The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?
title_full The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?
title_fullStr The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?
title_full_unstemmed The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense?
title_sort short-a nasal system: how does it get so tense?
publishDate 2014
url https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/
https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/1/CLA_poster_2014.pdf
https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/3/CLA_poster_2014.pdf
long_lat ENVELOPE(-168.583,-168.583,-84.933,-84.933)
ENVELOPE(22.034,22.034,69.974,69.974)
geographic Ferguson
Boberg
geographic_facet Ferguson
Boberg
genre Newfoundland
genre_facet Newfoundland
op_relation https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/1/CLA_poster_2014.pdf
https://research.library.mun.ca/6422/3/CLA_poster_2014.pdf
De Decker, Paul <https://research.library.mun.ca/view/creator_az/De_Decker=3APaul=3A=3A.html> (2014) The Short-a Nasal System: how does it get so tense? In: Canadian Linguistics Association, May 24–26, 2014, St. Catharine's, Ontario. (Unpublished)
_version_ 1778529611033870336