Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning

The presence of sex-stereotyped behavior in monomorphic animals, where there are no sexual differences in form to account for sexual differences in function, is often attributed to intraspecific competition or to differential parental investment. The possibility that the use of different behavioral...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:Behavioral Ecology
Main Authors: Elliott, Kyle Hamish, Gaston, Anthony J., Crump, Douglas
Format: Text
Language:English
Published: Oxford University Press 2010
Subjects:
Online Access:http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/arq076v1
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076
id fthighwire:oai:open-archive.highwire.org:beheco:arq076v1
record_format openpolar
spelling fthighwire:oai:open-archive.highwire.org:beheco:arq076v1 2023-05-15T18:41:33+02:00 Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning Elliott, Kyle Hamish Gaston, Anthony J. Crump, Douglas 2010-07-07 04:05:25.0 text/html http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/arq076v1 https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076 en eng Oxford University Press http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/arq076v1 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076 Copyright (C) 2010, International Society for Behavioral Ecology Article TEXT 2010 fthighwire https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076 2016-11-16T18:36:10Z The presence of sex-stereotyped behavior in monomorphic animals, where there are no sexual differences in form to account for sexual differences in function, is often attributed to intraspecific competition or to differential parental investment. The possibility that the use of different behavioral strategies by each parent may increase reproductive success for both partners through risk partitioning is seldom considered. We studied thick-billed murres ( Uria lomvia ), where the male exclusively feeds the offspring during the late chick rearing. During the period of biparental care, males fed on “risk-averse” prey (consistent across time and space; unitized risk = 0.29), whereas females fed on “risk-prone” prey (risk = 0.59). Males fed at night at 1 colony, during the day at 2 colonies, and there was no pattern at another colony. We suggest that these differences reflect the availability of risk-prone prey. Modeling suggested that mixed-risk pairs had higher success than “risky” or “riskless” pairs. Males accumulated reserves and reduced chick provisioning just prior to fledging. Thus, sex-specific patterns at 1 period (male-only care during postfledging) may have led to sex-specific patterns at earlier periods through the need for specialization in foraging habits and risk. We propose that risk partitioning may contribute to the prevalence of sex-specific behaviors in monomorphic animals and that patterns are likely context specific rather than species specific. Text Uria lomvia uria HighWire Press (Stanford University) Behavioral Ecology 21 5 1024 1032
institution Open Polar
collection HighWire Press (Stanford University)
op_collection_id fthighwire
language English
topic Article
spellingShingle Article
Elliott, Kyle Hamish
Gaston, Anthony J.
Crump, Douglas
Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
topic_facet Article
description The presence of sex-stereotyped behavior in monomorphic animals, where there are no sexual differences in form to account for sexual differences in function, is often attributed to intraspecific competition or to differential parental investment. The possibility that the use of different behavioral strategies by each parent may increase reproductive success for both partners through risk partitioning is seldom considered. We studied thick-billed murres ( Uria lomvia ), where the male exclusively feeds the offspring during the late chick rearing. During the period of biparental care, males fed on “risk-averse” prey (consistent across time and space; unitized risk = 0.29), whereas females fed on “risk-prone” prey (risk = 0.59). Males fed at night at 1 colony, during the day at 2 colonies, and there was no pattern at another colony. We suggest that these differences reflect the availability of risk-prone prey. Modeling suggested that mixed-risk pairs had higher success than “risky” or “riskless” pairs. Males accumulated reserves and reduced chick provisioning just prior to fledging. Thus, sex-specific patterns at 1 period (male-only care during postfledging) may have led to sex-specific patterns at earlier periods through the need for specialization in foraging habits and risk. We propose that risk partitioning may contribute to the prevalence of sex-specific behaviors in monomorphic animals and that patterns are likely context specific rather than species specific.
format Text
author Elliott, Kyle Hamish
Gaston, Anthony J.
Crump, Douglas
author_facet Elliott, Kyle Hamish
Gaston, Anthony J.
Crump, Douglas
author_sort Elliott, Kyle Hamish
title Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_short Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_full Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_fullStr Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_full_unstemmed Sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
title_sort sex-specific behavior by a monomorphic seabird represents risk partitioning
publisher Oxford University Press
publishDate 2010
url http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/arq076v1
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076
genre Uria lomvia
uria
genre_facet Uria lomvia
uria
op_relation http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/arq076v1
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076
op_rights Copyright (C) 2010, International Society for Behavioral Ecology
op_doi https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arq076
container_title Behavioral Ecology
container_volume 21
container_issue 5
container_start_page 1024
op_container_end_page 1032
_version_ 1766231092569833472