Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior

Adults of many species display extravagant sexual signals during the reproductive season, apparently evolved as a means of attracting mates or repelling potential competitors, thereby inadvertently also attracting the attention of predators. Many studies have shown predation costs of sexual display....

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:Behavioral Ecology
Main Authors: Møller, Anders Pape, Christiansen, Simon S., Mousseau, Timothy A.
Format: Text
Language:English
Published: Oxford University Press 2011
Subjects:
Online Access:http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/22/4/800
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046
id fthighwire:oai:open-archive.highwire.org:beheco:22/4/800
record_format openpolar
spelling fthighwire:oai:open-archive.highwire.org:beheco:22/4/800 2023-05-15T13:00:45+02:00 Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior Møller, Anders Pape Christiansen, Simon S. Mousseau, Timothy A. 2011-07-01 00:00:00.0 text/html http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/22/4/800 https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046 en eng Oxford University Press http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/22/4/800 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046 Copyright (C) 2011, International Society for Behavioral Ecology ORIGINAL ARTICLES TEXT 2011 fthighwire https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046 2016-11-16T17:34:56Z Adults of many species display extravagant sexual signals during the reproductive season, apparently evolved as a means of attracting mates or repelling potential competitors, thereby inadvertently also attracting the attention of predators. Many studies have shown predation costs of sexual display. Therefore, we should expect species with the most exaggerated signals to have evolved antipredator behavior that reduces or eliminates predation costs of sexual signaling but also to have evolved behavior that allows for escape from a predator once captured. We quantified 6 aspects of escape behavior in 2105 free-living birds belonging to 80 species when handled after capture for banding. Escape behavior was species specific as demonstrated by significant consistency in behavior among individuals. Escape behavior was significantly related to susceptibility to predation by cats Felis catus and goshawks Accipiter gentilis , showing that escape behavior is under current selection. Escape behavior was related to the ease of feather loss estimated in a previous study but also to the frequency of tailless individuals recorded in the field. Thus, escape behavior reported here was cross-validated against other aspects of antipredator behavior shown to reflect risk of predation. Aspects of escape behavior differed significantly between males and females (biting, fear screams, and feather loss). Sexually dichromatic species differed in escape behavior from monochromatic species by having a reduced frequency of fear screams and increased tonic immobility. These findings suggest that exposure to risk of predation has modified escape behavior in relation to sexual coloration. Text Accipiter gentilis HighWire Press (Stanford University) Behavioral Ecology 22 4 800 807
institution Open Polar
collection HighWire Press (Stanford University)
op_collection_id fthighwire
language English
topic ORIGINAL ARTICLES
spellingShingle ORIGINAL ARTICLES
Møller, Anders Pape
Christiansen, Simon S.
Mousseau, Timothy A.
Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
topic_facet ORIGINAL ARTICLES
description Adults of many species display extravagant sexual signals during the reproductive season, apparently evolved as a means of attracting mates or repelling potential competitors, thereby inadvertently also attracting the attention of predators. Many studies have shown predation costs of sexual display. Therefore, we should expect species with the most exaggerated signals to have evolved antipredator behavior that reduces or eliminates predation costs of sexual signaling but also to have evolved behavior that allows for escape from a predator once captured. We quantified 6 aspects of escape behavior in 2105 free-living birds belonging to 80 species when handled after capture for banding. Escape behavior was species specific as demonstrated by significant consistency in behavior among individuals. Escape behavior was significantly related to susceptibility to predation by cats Felis catus and goshawks Accipiter gentilis , showing that escape behavior is under current selection. Escape behavior was related to the ease of feather loss estimated in a previous study but also to the frequency of tailless individuals recorded in the field. Thus, escape behavior reported here was cross-validated against other aspects of antipredator behavior shown to reflect risk of predation. Aspects of escape behavior differed significantly between males and females (biting, fear screams, and feather loss). Sexually dichromatic species differed in escape behavior from monochromatic species by having a reduced frequency of fear screams and increased tonic immobility. These findings suggest that exposure to risk of predation has modified escape behavior in relation to sexual coloration.
format Text
author Møller, Anders Pape
Christiansen, Simon S.
Mousseau, Timothy A.
author_facet Møller, Anders Pape
Christiansen, Simon S.
Mousseau, Timothy A.
author_sort Møller, Anders Pape
title Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
title_short Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
title_full Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
title_fullStr Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
title_full_unstemmed Sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
title_sort sexual signals, risk of predation and escape behavior
publisher Oxford University Press
publishDate 2011
url http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/22/4/800
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046
genre Accipiter gentilis
genre_facet Accipiter gentilis
op_relation http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/22/4/800
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046
op_rights Copyright (C) 2011, International Society for Behavioral Ecology
op_doi https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr046
container_title Behavioral Ecology
container_volume 22
container_issue 4
container_start_page 800
op_container_end_page 807
_version_ 1766259247911272448