Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?

Social hierarchies result in the unequal distribution of resources, with dominant individuals able to monopolize access to food, shelter, and reproductive opportunities. However, the short-term benefits of priority access to resources have not always translated into long-term benefits in terms of gr...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:Behavioral Ecology
Main Authors: Harwood, Andrew J., Armstrong, John D., Metcalfe, Neil B., Griffiths, Siân W.
Format: Text
Language:English
Published: Oxford University Press 2003
Subjects:
Online Access:http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/14/6/902
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080
id fthighwire:oai:open-archive.highwire.org:beheco:14/6/902
record_format openpolar
spelling fthighwire:oai:open-archive.highwire.org:beheco:14/6/902 2023-05-15T15:31:41+02:00 Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)? Harwood, Andrew J. Armstrong, John D. Metcalfe, Neil B. Griffiths, Siân W. 2003-11-01 00:00:00.0 text/html http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/14/6/902 https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080 en eng Oxford University Press http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/14/6/902 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080 Copyright (C) 2003, International Society for Behavioral Ecology Articles TEXT 2003 fthighwire https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080 2016-11-16T17:16:15Z Social hierarchies result in the unequal distribution of resources, with dominant individuals able to monopolize access to food, shelter, and reproductive opportunities. However, the short-term benefits of priority access to resources have not always translated into long-term benefits in terms of growth and survival. In the present study, we test whether dominant Atlantic salmon ( Salmo salar ) that were able to monopolize a food source in laboratory conditions had a growth advantage over subordinates in their natural stream. There was no relationship between initial size and rank, and high-ranking individuals showed no growth advantage over subordinates over a 2-month period when returned to the wild. A fish's growth rate in the wild was also unrelated to its sex or initial size, or the density of other salmon of the same age class within each experimental site. There was, however, spatial variability in growth, with salmon in one site gaining twice as much weight as did fish from the other sites. This suggests that at most of the sites, resources were limited in availability and that the absence of a relationship between growth and dominance rank was not owing simply to an excess of food being available. The lack of a positive correlation between social status and growth in the wild may be explained by several mechanisms, including the spatio-temporal variability in resources, interspecific interactions, fluctuations in habitat, or the presence of predators. Text Atlantic salmon Salmo salar HighWire Press (Stanford University) Behavioral Ecology 14 6 902 908
institution Open Polar
collection HighWire Press (Stanford University)
op_collection_id fthighwire
language English
topic Articles
spellingShingle Articles
Harwood, Andrew J.
Armstrong, John D.
Metcalfe, Neil B.
Griffiths, Siân W.
Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?
topic_facet Articles
description Social hierarchies result in the unequal distribution of resources, with dominant individuals able to monopolize access to food, shelter, and reproductive opportunities. However, the short-term benefits of priority access to resources have not always translated into long-term benefits in terms of growth and survival. In the present study, we test whether dominant Atlantic salmon ( Salmo salar ) that were able to monopolize a food source in laboratory conditions had a growth advantage over subordinates in their natural stream. There was no relationship between initial size and rank, and high-ranking individuals showed no growth advantage over subordinates over a 2-month period when returned to the wild. A fish's growth rate in the wild was also unrelated to its sex or initial size, or the density of other salmon of the same age class within each experimental site. There was, however, spatial variability in growth, with salmon in one site gaining twice as much weight as did fish from the other sites. This suggests that at most of the sites, resources were limited in availability and that the absence of a relationship between growth and dominance rank was not owing simply to an excess of food being available. The lack of a positive correlation between social status and growth in the wild may be explained by several mechanisms, including the spatio-temporal variability in resources, interspecific interactions, fluctuations in habitat, or the presence of predators.
format Text
author Harwood, Andrew J.
Armstrong, John D.
Metcalfe, Neil B.
Griffiths, Siân W.
author_facet Harwood, Andrew J.
Armstrong, John D.
Metcalfe, Neil B.
Griffiths, Siân W.
author_sort Harwood, Andrew J.
title Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?
title_short Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?
title_full Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?
title_fullStr Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?
title_full_unstemmed Does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling Atlantic salmon (Salmo salar)?
title_sort does dominance status correlate with growth in wild stream-dwelling atlantic salmon (salmo salar)?
publisher Oxford University Press
publishDate 2003
url http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/14/6/902
https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080
genre Atlantic salmon
Salmo salar
genre_facet Atlantic salmon
Salmo salar
op_relation http://beheco.oxfordjournals.org/cgi/content/short/14/6/902
http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080
op_rights Copyright (C) 2003, International Society for Behavioral Ecology
op_doi https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arg080
container_title Behavioral Ecology
container_volume 14
container_issue 6
container_start_page 902
op_container_end_page 908
_version_ 1766362211044818944