Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey

Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pu rsuits. If echoes are processed on a click by click basis, as assumed...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Vance, Heather, Madsen, Peter, Aguilar de Soto, Natacha, Wisniewska, Danuta, Ladegaard, Michael, Hooker, Sascha, Johnson, Mark
Format: Article in Journal/Newspaper
Language:unknown
Published: Zenodo 2021
Subjects:
Online Access:https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175843
https://zenodo.org/record/5175843
id ftdatacite:10.5281/zenodo.5175843
record_format openpolar
spelling ftdatacite:10.5281/zenodo.5175843 2023-05-15T16:33:18+02:00 Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey Vance, Heather Madsen, Peter Aguilar de Soto, Natacha Wisniewska, Danuta Ladegaard, Michael Hooker, Sascha Johnson, Mark 2021 https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175843 https://zenodo.org/record/5175843 unknown Zenodo https://zenodo.org/communities/dryad https://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n8pk0p2w1 https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175844 https://zenodo.org/communities/dryad Open Access Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode cc-by-4.0 info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess CC-BY Echolocation toothed-whales predator-prey interactions response latency Harbour Porpoise Blainville's beaked whale Software SoftwareSourceCode article 2021 ftdatacite https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175843 https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n8pk0p2w1 https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175844 2021-11-05T12:55:41Z Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pu rsuits. If echoes are processed on a click by click basis, as assumed, neural responses 100x faster than those in vision are required to keep pace with this information flow. Using high resolution bio-logging of wild predator prey interactions we show that toothed whales adjust clicking rates to track prey movement within 50 200 ms of prey escape responses. Hypothesising that these stereotyped biosonar adjustments are elicited by sudden prey accelerations, we measured echo kinetic responses from trained harb our porpoises to a moving target and found similar latencies. High biosonar sampling rates are, therefore, not supported by extreme speeds of neural processing and muscular responses. Instead, the neuro kinetic response times in echolocation are similar to those of tracking responses in vision, suggesting a common neural underpinning. : This data set is divided into wild harbour porpoise data (files beginning with hp), wild beaked whale data (files beginning with md) and trained harbour porpoise data (files beginning with either Freja17 or Sif17). The wild data represent the echolocation responses of harbour porpoise and beaked whales to their prey in the wild. The trained harbour porpoise data represents echolocation responses to a moving aluminium sphere target in a captive setting. Each file contains an individual prey encounter/moving target trial. The data is presented in a NetCDF format and includes additional metadata within each data structure. The structures included are 1) X -the sound envelope 2) CL - the timing of echolocation clicks relative to the start of the sound envelope 3) R- the time of prey/target movement relative to start of sound extract and distance from prey/target during movement 4) info- an information structure for the tag deployment and 5) for trained harbour porpoise trials only, A - the two axes acceleration of the aluminum target sphere. We have further included several MATLAB scripts which will allow users to plot echograms of the acoustic data, the same as those presented in Vance et al (2021). Additional information on how to use these is included within the help information for each script. Funding provided by: Bundesamt für Naturschutz Crossref Funder Registry ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100010415 Award Number: Z1.2 5330/2010/14 : See 'Materials and Methods' section of Vance et al (2021). Article in Journal/Newspaper Harbour porpoise toothed whales DataCite Metadata Store (German National Library of Science and Technology) Vance ENVELOPE(-139.567,-139.567,-75.467,-75.467)
institution Open Polar
collection DataCite Metadata Store (German National Library of Science and Technology)
op_collection_id ftdatacite
language unknown
topic Echolocation
toothed-whales
predator-prey interactions
response latency
Harbour Porpoise
Blainville's beaked whale
spellingShingle Echolocation
toothed-whales
predator-prey interactions
response latency
Harbour Porpoise
Blainville's beaked whale
Vance, Heather
Madsen, Peter
Aguilar de Soto, Natacha
Wisniewska, Danuta
Ladegaard, Michael
Hooker, Sascha
Johnson, Mark
Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
topic_facet Echolocation
toothed-whales
predator-prey interactions
response latency
Harbour Porpoise
Blainville's beaked whale
description Visual predators rely on fast-acting optokinetic responses to track and capture agile prey. Most toothed whales, however, rely on echolocation for hunting and have converged on biosonar clicking rates reaching 500/s during prey pu rsuits. If echoes are processed on a click by click basis, as assumed, neural responses 100x faster than those in vision are required to keep pace with this information flow. Using high resolution bio-logging of wild predator prey interactions we show that toothed whales adjust clicking rates to track prey movement within 50 200 ms of prey escape responses. Hypothesising that these stereotyped biosonar adjustments are elicited by sudden prey accelerations, we measured echo kinetic responses from trained harb our porpoises to a moving target and found similar latencies. High biosonar sampling rates are, therefore, not supported by extreme speeds of neural processing and muscular responses. Instead, the neuro kinetic response times in echolocation are similar to those of tracking responses in vision, suggesting a common neural underpinning. : This data set is divided into wild harbour porpoise data (files beginning with hp), wild beaked whale data (files beginning with md) and trained harbour porpoise data (files beginning with either Freja17 or Sif17). The wild data represent the echolocation responses of harbour porpoise and beaked whales to their prey in the wild. The trained harbour porpoise data represents echolocation responses to a moving aluminium sphere target in a captive setting. Each file contains an individual prey encounter/moving target trial. The data is presented in a NetCDF format and includes additional metadata within each data structure. The structures included are 1) X -the sound envelope 2) CL - the timing of echolocation clicks relative to the start of the sound envelope 3) R- the time of prey/target movement relative to start of sound extract and distance from prey/target during movement 4) info- an information structure for the tag deployment and 5) for trained harbour porpoise trials only, A - the two axes acceleration of the aluminum target sphere. We have further included several MATLAB scripts which will allow users to plot echograms of the acoustic data, the same as those presented in Vance et al (2021). Additional information on how to use these is included within the help information for each script. Funding provided by: Bundesamt für Naturschutz Crossref Funder Registry ID: http://dx.doi.org/10.13039/501100010415 Award Number: Z1.2 5330/2010/14 : See 'Materials and Methods' section of Vance et al (2021).
format Article in Journal/Newspaper
author Vance, Heather
Madsen, Peter
Aguilar de Soto, Natacha
Wisniewska, Danuta
Ladegaard, Michael
Hooker, Sascha
Johnson, Mark
author_facet Vance, Heather
Madsen, Peter
Aguilar de Soto, Natacha
Wisniewska, Danuta
Ladegaard, Michael
Hooker, Sascha
Johnson, Mark
author_sort Vance, Heather
title Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
title_short Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
title_full Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
title_fullStr Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
title_full_unstemmed Echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
title_sort echolocating toothed whales use ultra-fast echo-kinetic responses to track evasive prey
publisher Zenodo
publishDate 2021
url https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175843
https://zenodo.org/record/5175843
long_lat ENVELOPE(-139.567,-139.567,-75.467,-75.467)
geographic Vance
geographic_facet Vance
genre Harbour porpoise
toothed whales
genre_facet Harbour porpoise
toothed whales
op_relation https://zenodo.org/communities/dryad
https://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n8pk0p2w1
https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175844
https://zenodo.org/communities/dryad
op_rights Open Access
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode
cc-by-4.0
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
op_rightsnorm CC-BY
op_doi https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175843
https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.n8pk0p2w1
https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.5175844
_version_ 1766022996231716864