Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics

We have studied the temporal characteristics of bistable perception of the stimuli of two types: one involves alterations in a perceived depth and another one has an ambiguous content. We used the Necker lattice and lines of shadowed circles ambiguously perceived either as spheres or holes as stimul...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Podvigina, Daria N., Chernigovskaya, Tatiana V.
Format: Text
Language:English
Published: Zenodo 2015
Subjects:
Online Access:https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110348
https://zenodo.org/record/1110348
id ftdatacite:10.5281/zenodo.1110348
record_format openpolar
institution Open Polar
collection DataCite Metadata Store (German National Library of Science and Technology)
op_collection_id ftdatacite
language English
topic Multistable perception
perceived depth
reversal rate
top-down processes.
spellingShingle Multistable perception
perceived depth
reversal rate
top-down processes.
Podvigina, Daria N.
Chernigovskaya, Tatiana V.
Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics
topic_facet Multistable perception
perceived depth
reversal rate
top-down processes.
description We have studied the temporal characteristics of bistable perception of the stimuli of two types: one involves alterations in a perceived depth and another one has an ambiguous content. We used the Necker lattice and lines of shadowed circles ambiguously perceived either as spheres or holes as stimuli of the first type. The Winson figure (the Eskimo/Indian picture) was a stimulus of the second type. We have analyzed how often the reversals occurred (reversal rate) and for how long each of the two interpretations, or percepts, was observed during one presentation (stability durations). For all three ambiguous images the reversal rate and the stability durations had similar values, which provide another evidence for a significant role of top-down processes in multistable perception. : {"references": ["J. Kornmeier, and M. Bach, \"Ambiguous figures \u2013 what happens in the\nbrain when perception changes but not the stimulus,\" Front. Hum.\nNeurosci., 2012, vol. 6, pp, 1-23.", "T.C. Toppino, and G.M. Long, \"Selective adaptation with reversible\nfigures: don't change that channel\", Percept Psychophys., 1987, vol. 42,\npp. 37\u201348.", "G.M. Long, and A.D. Olszweski, \"To reverse or not to reverse: when is\nan ambiguous figure not ambiguous?\", Am J Psychol., 1999, vol. 112,\npp. 41\u201371.", "J. Kornmeier, and M. Bach, \"Early neural activity in Necker-cube\nreversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon\",\nPsychophysiology, 2004, vol. 41, iss. 1, pp. 1-8.", "I. Rock, S. Hall, and J. Davis, \"Why do ambiguous figures reverse,\"\nActa Psychologica, 1994, vol. 87, iss. 1, pp. 33\u201359.", "D.A. Leiopold, and N.K. Logothetis, \"Multistable phenomena: changing\nviews in perception,\" Trends Cogn. Sci., 1999, vol. 3, iss. 7, 1 July, pp.\n254\u2013264.", "J. Kornmeier, Ch.M. Hein, and M.Bach, \"Multistable perception: When\nbottom-up and top-down coincide,\" Brain and Cognition, 2009, vol. 69,\npp. 138-147.", "R. van Ee, L.C.J. van Dam, G.J. Brouwer, \"Voluntary control and the\ndynamics of perceptual bi-stability\", Vis Res, 2005, vol 45, pp. 41-55.", "J. Kornmeier, S.P. Heinrich, H. Atmanspacher, and M. Bach, \"The\nreversing \"Necker Wall\" - a new paradigm with reversal entrainment\nreveals an early EEG correlate,\" Investigative Ophthalmology and\nVisual Science, 2001, vol. 42, p. 409.\n[10] J.E. Bergum, B.O. Bergum, \"Self-perceived creativity and ambiguous\nfigure reversal-rates\", Bull Psychonom Soc, 1979, vol. 14, no 5, pp. 373-\n374.\n[11] B.O. Bergum, J.E. Bergum, \"Creativity, perceptual stability, and selfperception\",\nBull Psychonom Soc, 1979, vol. 14, no 1, pp. 61\u201365.\n[12] H. Klintman, \"Original thinking and ambiguous figure reversal rates\",\nBull Psychonom Soc, 1984, vol. 22, no 2, pp. 129\u2013131.\n[13] J. Wernery, \"Bistable perception of the Necker cube in the context of\ncognition and personality\", ETH, 2013.\n[14] N.F. Troje, and M. McAdam, \"The viewing-from-above bias and the\nsilhoutte illusion\", i-Perception, 2010, vol. 1, iss. 3, pp. 143\u2013148.\n[15] V.S. Ramachandran, \"Perceiving Shape from Shading\", Sci. Amer.,\n1988, vol. 259. no 2, pp. 76-83.\n[16] V.A. Maksimova, D.N. Podvigina, \"Ambiguous figures perception\nunder binocular and monocular viewing conditions\", Perception, 2014,\nvol 43, ECVP Abstract Supplement, p. 111."]}
format Text
author Podvigina, Daria N.
Chernigovskaya, Tatiana V.
author_facet Podvigina, Daria N.
Chernigovskaya, Tatiana V.
author_sort Podvigina, Daria N.
title Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics
title_short Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics
title_full Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics
title_fullStr Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics
title_full_unstemmed Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics
title_sort top-down influences to multistable perception: evidence from temporal dynamics
publisher Zenodo
publishDate 2015
url https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110348
https://zenodo.org/record/1110348
long_lat ENVELOPE(18.294,18.294,69.593,69.593)
ENVELOPE(66.250,66.250,-72.833,-72.833)
geographic Bergum
Indian
Maksimova
geographic_facet Bergum
Indian
Maksimova
genre eskimo*
genre_facet eskimo*
op_relation https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110349
op_rights Open Access
Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0
info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess
op_rightsnorm CC-BY
op_doi https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110348
https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110349
_version_ 1766402815143444480
spelling ftdatacite:10.5281/zenodo.1110348 2023-05-15T16:06:47+02:00 Top-Down Influences To Multistable Perception: Evidence From Temporal Dynamics Podvigina, Daria N. Chernigovskaya, Tatiana V. 2015 https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110348 https://zenodo.org/record/1110348 en eng Zenodo https://dx.doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110349 Open Access Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0 info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess CC-BY Multistable perception perceived depth reversal rate top-down processes. Text Journal article article-journal ScholarlyArticle 2015 ftdatacite https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110348 https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1110349 2021-11-05T12:55:41Z We have studied the temporal characteristics of bistable perception of the stimuli of two types: one involves alterations in a perceived depth and another one has an ambiguous content. We used the Necker lattice and lines of shadowed circles ambiguously perceived either as spheres or holes as stimuli of the first type. The Winson figure (the Eskimo/Indian picture) was a stimulus of the second type. We have analyzed how often the reversals occurred (reversal rate) and for how long each of the two interpretations, or percepts, was observed during one presentation (stability durations). For all three ambiguous images the reversal rate and the stability durations had similar values, which provide another evidence for a significant role of top-down processes in multistable perception. : {"references": ["J. Kornmeier, and M. Bach, \"Ambiguous figures \u2013 what happens in the\nbrain when perception changes but not the stimulus,\" Front. Hum.\nNeurosci., 2012, vol. 6, pp, 1-23.", "T.C. Toppino, and G.M. Long, \"Selective adaptation with reversible\nfigures: don't change that channel\", Percept Psychophys., 1987, vol. 42,\npp. 37\u201348.", "G.M. Long, and A.D. Olszweski, \"To reverse or not to reverse: when is\nan ambiguous figure not ambiguous?\", Am J Psychol., 1999, vol. 112,\npp. 41\u201371.", "J. Kornmeier, and M. Bach, \"Early neural activity in Necker-cube\nreversal: evidence for low-level processing of a gestalt phenomenon\",\nPsychophysiology, 2004, vol. 41, iss. 1, pp. 1-8.", "I. Rock, S. Hall, and J. Davis, \"Why do ambiguous figures reverse,\"\nActa Psychologica, 1994, vol. 87, iss. 1, pp. 33\u201359.", "D.A. Leiopold, and N.K. Logothetis, \"Multistable phenomena: changing\nviews in perception,\" Trends Cogn. Sci., 1999, vol. 3, iss. 7, 1 July, pp.\n254\u2013264.", "J. Kornmeier, Ch.M. Hein, and M.Bach, \"Multistable perception: When\nbottom-up and top-down coincide,\" Brain and Cognition, 2009, vol. 69,\npp. 138-147.", "R. van Ee, L.C.J. van Dam, G.J. Brouwer, \"Voluntary control and the\ndynamics of perceptual bi-stability\", Vis Res, 2005, vol 45, pp. 41-55.", "J. Kornmeier, S.P. Heinrich, H. Atmanspacher, and M. Bach, \"The\nreversing \"Necker Wall\" - a new paradigm with reversal entrainment\nreveals an early EEG correlate,\" Investigative Ophthalmology and\nVisual Science, 2001, vol. 42, p. 409.\n[10] J.E. Bergum, B.O. Bergum, \"Self-perceived creativity and ambiguous\nfigure reversal-rates\", Bull Psychonom Soc, 1979, vol. 14, no 5, pp. 373-\n374.\n[11] B.O. Bergum, J.E. Bergum, \"Creativity, perceptual stability, and selfperception\",\nBull Psychonom Soc, 1979, vol. 14, no 1, pp. 61\u201365.\n[12] H. Klintman, \"Original thinking and ambiguous figure reversal rates\",\nBull Psychonom Soc, 1984, vol. 22, no 2, pp. 129\u2013131.\n[13] J. Wernery, \"Bistable perception of the Necker cube in the context of\ncognition and personality\", ETH, 2013.\n[14] N.F. Troje, and M. McAdam, \"The viewing-from-above bias and the\nsilhoutte illusion\", i-Perception, 2010, vol. 1, iss. 3, pp. 143\u2013148.\n[15] V.S. Ramachandran, \"Perceiving Shape from Shading\", Sci. Amer.,\n1988, vol. 259. no 2, pp. 76-83.\n[16] V.A. Maksimova, D.N. Podvigina, \"Ambiguous figures perception\nunder binocular and monocular viewing conditions\", Perception, 2014,\nvol 43, ECVP Abstract Supplement, p. 111."]} Text eskimo* DataCite Metadata Store (German National Library of Science and Technology) Bergum ENVELOPE(18.294,18.294,69.593,69.593) Indian Maksimova ENVELOPE(66.250,66.250,-72.833,-72.833)