Individuals and Their Concepts

Nearly all research on concepts in cognitive psychology is research on categories of objects — categories of teapots or turnips, for example. But when it comes to things that are important to us — people, pets, works of art, special places — we also represent the individuals themselves, not just the...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Sergey Blok, George Newman, Lance J. Rips
Other Authors: The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives
Format: Text
Language:English
Subjects:
Online Access:http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270
http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf
id ftciteseerx:oai:CiteSeerX.psu:10.1.1.130.3270
record_format openpolar
spelling ftciteseerx:oai:CiteSeerX.psu:10.1.1.130.3270 2023-05-15T16:35:29+02:00 Individuals and Their Concepts Sergey Blok George Newman Lance J. Rips The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives application/pdf http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf en eng http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf Metadata may be used without restrictions as long as the oai identifier remains attached to it. http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf text ftciteseerx 2016-01-07T14:30:43Z Nearly all research on concepts in cognitive psychology is research on categories of objects — categories of teapots or turnips, for example. But when it comes to things that are important to us — people, pets, works of art, special places — we also represent the individuals themselves, not just the categories they belong to. Proper nouns, such as Herman Melville, Toto, Broadway Boogie-Woogie, or Hudson Bay, can denote these individuals, but you can also represent individuals for whom you have no conventional names, like the bed you usually sleep in or your neighbor’s mulberry tree. It is Doug Medin who is mainly responsible for calling category researchers ’ attention to the importance of individual concepts. Medin and Schaffer’s (1978) Context Model proposed that much of what we know about categories we determine from our memories of their exemplars. It’s hard to overestimate the importance of this model: Not only has it produced generations of similar theories of categorization (e.g., Kruschke, 1992; Nosofsky, 1986), but it has also influenced fields as diverse as the psychology of attention (Logan, 2002), social psychology (Smith & Zarate, 1992), and phonology information that concepts afford (e.g., Medin, 1989; Medin & Ortony, 1989), but he still retains a fondness for exemplars. In fact, this tension in Doug’s thinking about concepts is characteristic of a special turn of mind, a form of reasoning that we’re tempted to call “modus medins ” and that the following schema approximates: Text Hudson Bay Unknown Hudson Hudson Bay
institution Open Polar
collection Unknown
op_collection_id ftciteseerx
language English
description Nearly all research on concepts in cognitive psychology is research on categories of objects — categories of teapots or turnips, for example. But when it comes to things that are important to us — people, pets, works of art, special places — we also represent the individuals themselves, not just the categories they belong to. Proper nouns, such as Herman Melville, Toto, Broadway Boogie-Woogie, or Hudson Bay, can denote these individuals, but you can also represent individuals for whom you have no conventional names, like the bed you usually sleep in or your neighbor’s mulberry tree. It is Doug Medin who is mainly responsible for calling category researchers ’ attention to the importance of individual concepts. Medin and Schaffer’s (1978) Context Model proposed that much of what we know about categories we determine from our memories of their exemplars. It’s hard to overestimate the importance of this model: Not only has it produced generations of similar theories of categorization (e.g., Kruschke, 1992; Nosofsky, 1986), but it has also influenced fields as diverse as the psychology of attention (Logan, 2002), social psychology (Smith & Zarate, 1992), and phonology information that concepts afford (e.g., Medin, 1989; Medin & Ortony, 1989), but he still retains a fondness for exemplars. In fact, this tension in Doug’s thinking about concepts is characteristic of a special turn of mind, a form of reasoning that we’re tempted to call “modus medins ” and that the following schema approximates:
author2 The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives
format Text
author Sergey Blok
George Newman
Lance J. Rips
spellingShingle Sergey Blok
George Newman
Lance J. Rips
Individuals and Their Concepts
author_facet Sergey Blok
George Newman
Lance J. Rips
author_sort Sergey Blok
title Individuals and Their Concepts
title_short Individuals and Their Concepts
title_full Individuals and Their Concepts
title_fullStr Individuals and Their Concepts
title_full_unstemmed Individuals and Their Concepts
title_sort individuals and their concepts
url http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270
http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf
geographic Hudson
Hudson Bay
geographic_facet Hudson
Hudson Bay
genre Hudson Bay
genre_facet Hudson Bay
op_source http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf
op_relation http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270
http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf
op_rights Metadata may be used without restrictions as long as the oai identifier remains attached to it.
_version_ 1766025702486835200