Individuals and Their Concepts
Nearly all research on concepts in cognitive psychology is research on categories of objects — categories of teapots or turnips, for example. But when it comes to things that are important to us — people, pets, works of art, special places — we also represent the individuals themselves, not just the...
Main Authors: | , , |
---|---|
Other Authors: | |
Format: | Text |
Language: | English |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf |
id |
ftciteseerx:oai:CiteSeerX.psu:10.1.1.130.3270 |
---|---|
record_format |
openpolar |
spelling |
ftciteseerx:oai:CiteSeerX.psu:10.1.1.130.3270 2023-05-15T16:35:29+02:00 Individuals and Their Concepts Sergey Blok George Newman Lance J. Rips The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives application/pdf http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf en eng http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf Metadata may be used without restrictions as long as the oai identifier remains attached to it. http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf text ftciteseerx 2016-01-07T14:30:43Z Nearly all research on concepts in cognitive psychology is research on categories of objects — categories of teapots or turnips, for example. But when it comes to things that are important to us — people, pets, works of art, special places — we also represent the individuals themselves, not just the categories they belong to. Proper nouns, such as Herman Melville, Toto, Broadway Boogie-Woogie, or Hudson Bay, can denote these individuals, but you can also represent individuals for whom you have no conventional names, like the bed you usually sleep in or your neighbor’s mulberry tree. It is Doug Medin who is mainly responsible for calling category researchers ’ attention to the importance of individual concepts. Medin and Schaffer’s (1978) Context Model proposed that much of what we know about categories we determine from our memories of their exemplars. It’s hard to overestimate the importance of this model: Not only has it produced generations of similar theories of categorization (e.g., Kruschke, 1992; Nosofsky, 1986), but it has also influenced fields as diverse as the psychology of attention (Logan, 2002), social psychology (Smith & Zarate, 1992), and phonology information that concepts afford (e.g., Medin, 1989; Medin & Ortony, 1989), but he still retains a fondness for exemplars. In fact, this tension in Doug’s thinking about concepts is characteristic of a special turn of mind, a form of reasoning that we’re tempted to call “modus medins ” and that the following schema approximates: Text Hudson Bay Unknown Hudson Hudson Bay |
institution |
Open Polar |
collection |
Unknown |
op_collection_id |
ftciteseerx |
language |
English |
description |
Nearly all research on concepts in cognitive psychology is research on categories of objects — categories of teapots or turnips, for example. But when it comes to things that are important to us — people, pets, works of art, special places — we also represent the individuals themselves, not just the categories they belong to. Proper nouns, such as Herman Melville, Toto, Broadway Boogie-Woogie, or Hudson Bay, can denote these individuals, but you can also represent individuals for whom you have no conventional names, like the bed you usually sleep in or your neighbor’s mulberry tree. It is Doug Medin who is mainly responsible for calling category researchers ’ attention to the importance of individual concepts. Medin and Schaffer’s (1978) Context Model proposed that much of what we know about categories we determine from our memories of their exemplars. It’s hard to overestimate the importance of this model: Not only has it produced generations of similar theories of categorization (e.g., Kruschke, 1992; Nosofsky, 1986), but it has also influenced fields as diverse as the psychology of attention (Logan, 2002), social psychology (Smith & Zarate, 1992), and phonology information that concepts afford (e.g., Medin, 1989; Medin & Ortony, 1989), but he still retains a fondness for exemplars. In fact, this tension in Doug’s thinking about concepts is characteristic of a special turn of mind, a form of reasoning that we’re tempted to call “modus medins ” and that the following schema approximates: |
author2 |
The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives |
format |
Text |
author |
Sergey Blok George Newman Lance J. Rips |
spellingShingle |
Sergey Blok George Newman Lance J. Rips Individuals and Their Concepts |
author_facet |
Sergey Blok George Newman Lance J. Rips |
author_sort |
Sergey Blok |
title |
Individuals and Their Concepts |
title_short |
Individuals and Their Concepts |
title_full |
Individuals and Their Concepts |
title_fullStr |
Individuals and Their Concepts |
title_full_unstemmed |
Individuals and Their Concepts |
title_sort |
individuals and their concepts |
url |
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf |
geographic |
Hudson Hudson Bay |
geographic_facet |
Hudson Hudson Bay |
genre |
Hudson Bay |
genre_facet |
Hudson Bay |
op_source |
http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf |
op_relation |
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.130.3270 http://mental.psych.northwestern.edu/assets/indivchap.pdf |
op_rights |
Metadata may be used without restrictions as long as the oai identifier remains attached to it. |
_version_ |
1766025702486835200 |