Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents

Abstract New referents are typically introduced into adult discourse with lexical nouns. This makes new referents maximally clear for listeners, and helps the listeners direct their attention appropriately. A different trend is observed in child language, where new referents may be realised with dem...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Barbora Skarabela, Shanley E M Allen, Thomas C Scott-Phillips
Other Authors: The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives
Format: Text
Language:English
Published: 2013
Subjects:
Online Access:http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.1072.7805
id ftciteseerx:oai:CiteSeerX.psu:10.1.1.1072.7805
record_format openpolar
spelling ftciteseerx:oai:CiteSeerX.psu:10.1.1.1072.7805 2023-05-15T16:55:36+02:00 Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents Barbora Skarabela Shanley E M Allen Thomas C Scott-Phillips The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives 2013 application/pdf http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.1072.7805 en eng http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.1072.7805 Metadata may be used without restrictions as long as the oai identifier remains attached to it. https://www.sowi.uni-kl.de/fileadmin/ewiss/pdf/Joint_attention_helps_explain_why_children_omit_new_referents__2_.pdf text 2013 ftciteseerx 2020-04-26T00:24:47Z Abstract New referents are typically introduced into adult discourse with lexical nouns. This makes new referents maximally clear for listeners, and helps the listeners direct their attention appropriately. A different trend is observed in child language, where new referents may be realised with demonstratives or pronouns, or they may be omitted altogether. This has led some to claim that children are pragmatically immature and not sensitive to the perspective of their interlocutors. In this paper, we analyse a videotaped corpus of naturalistic spontaneous speech of four children acquiring Inuktitut (2;0--3;6) to examine the different ways in which they realise new referents. Our results show that in their realisation of new referents children are sensitive to the presence or absence of joint attention. Specifically, they tend to omit arguments when joint attention is present, and they use lexical forms when it is absent. Their use of demonstratives reflects similar sensitivity: they tend to use demonstrative clitics when joint attention is present but independent demonstrative forms when it is absent. The use of omitted forms in child language is thus not explained by any pragmatic deficiency; indeed, it shows that children adjust their messages for the interlocutor, strictly following the Gricean Maxim of Quantity. Text inuktitut Unknown
institution Open Polar
collection Unknown
op_collection_id ftciteseerx
language English
description Abstract New referents are typically introduced into adult discourse with lexical nouns. This makes new referents maximally clear for listeners, and helps the listeners direct their attention appropriately. A different trend is observed in child language, where new referents may be realised with demonstratives or pronouns, or they may be omitted altogether. This has led some to claim that children are pragmatically immature and not sensitive to the perspective of their interlocutors. In this paper, we analyse a videotaped corpus of naturalistic spontaneous speech of four children acquiring Inuktitut (2;0--3;6) to examine the different ways in which they realise new referents. Our results show that in their realisation of new referents children are sensitive to the presence or absence of joint attention. Specifically, they tend to omit arguments when joint attention is present, and they use lexical forms when it is absent. Their use of demonstratives reflects similar sensitivity: they tend to use demonstrative clitics when joint attention is present but independent demonstrative forms when it is absent. The use of omitted forms in child language is thus not explained by any pragmatic deficiency; indeed, it shows that children adjust their messages for the interlocutor, strictly following the Gricean Maxim of Quantity.
author2 The Pennsylvania State University CiteSeerX Archives
format Text
author Barbora Skarabela
Shanley E M Allen
Thomas C Scott-Phillips
spellingShingle Barbora Skarabela
Shanley E M Allen
Thomas C Scott-Phillips
Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
author_facet Barbora Skarabela
Shanley E M Allen
Thomas C Scott-Phillips
author_sort Barbora Skarabela
title Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
title_short Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
title_full Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
title_fullStr Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
title_full_unstemmed Joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
title_sort joint attention helps explain why children omit new referents
publishDate 2013
url http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.1072.7805
genre inuktitut
genre_facet inuktitut
op_source https://www.sowi.uni-kl.de/fileadmin/ewiss/pdf/Joint_attention_helps_explain_why_children_omit_new_referents__2_.pdf
op_relation http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/summary?doi=10.1.1.1072.7805
op_rights Metadata may be used without restrictions as long as the oai identifier remains attached to it.
_version_ 1766046589350051840