Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited

© 2016 Regier et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. The claim that Eskimo languages have words...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:PLOS ONE
Main Authors: Regier, T, Carstensen, A, Kemp, C
Format: Article in Journal/Newspaper
Language:English
Published: eScholarship, University of California 2016
Subjects:
Online Access:http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d
id ftcdlib:qt2qb5722d
record_format openpolar
spelling ftcdlib:qt2qb5722d 2023-05-15T16:07:04+02:00 Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited Regier, T Carstensen, A Kemp, C e0151138 2016-04-01 application/pdf http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d english eng eScholarship, University of California qt2qb5722d http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d public Regier, T; Carstensen, A; & Kemp, C. (2016). Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0151138. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0151138. UC Berkeley: Retrieved from: http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d article 2016 ftcdlib https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151138 2018-07-13T22:56:25Z © 2016 Regier et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. The claim that Eskimo languages have words for different types of snow is well-known among the public, but has been greatly exaggerated through popularization and is therefore viewed with skepticism by many scholars of language. Despite the prominence of this claim, to our knowledge the line of reasoning behind it has not been tested broadly across languages. Here, we note that this reasoning is a special case of the more general view that language is shaped by the need for efficient communication, and we empirically test a variant of it against multiple sources of data, including library reference works, Twitter, and large digital collections of linguistic and meteorological data. Consistent with the hypothesis of efficient communication, we find that languages that use the same linguistic form for snow and ice tend to be spoken in warmer climates, and that this association appears to be mediated by lower communicative need to talk about snow and ice. Our results confirm that variation in semantic categories across languages may be traceable in part to local communicative needs. They suggest moreover that despite its awkward history, the topic of "words for snow" may play a useful role as an accessible instance of the principle that language supports efficient communication. Article in Journal/Newspaper eskimo* University of California: eScholarship PLOS ONE 11 4 e0151138
institution Open Polar
collection University of California: eScholarship
op_collection_id ftcdlib
language English
description © 2016 Regier et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. The claim that Eskimo languages have words for different types of snow is well-known among the public, but has been greatly exaggerated through popularization and is therefore viewed with skepticism by many scholars of language. Despite the prominence of this claim, to our knowledge the line of reasoning behind it has not been tested broadly across languages. Here, we note that this reasoning is a special case of the more general view that language is shaped by the need for efficient communication, and we empirically test a variant of it against multiple sources of data, including library reference works, Twitter, and large digital collections of linguistic and meteorological data. Consistent with the hypothesis of efficient communication, we find that languages that use the same linguistic form for snow and ice tend to be spoken in warmer climates, and that this association appears to be mediated by lower communicative need to talk about snow and ice. Our results confirm that variation in semantic categories across languages may be traceable in part to local communicative needs. They suggest moreover that despite its awkward history, the topic of "words for snow" may play a useful role as an accessible instance of the principle that language supports efficient communication.
format Article in Journal/Newspaper
author Regier, T
Carstensen, A
Kemp, C
spellingShingle Regier, T
Carstensen, A
Kemp, C
Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited
author_facet Regier, T
Carstensen, A
Kemp, C
author_sort Regier, T
title Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited
title_short Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited
title_full Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited
title_fullStr Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited
title_full_unstemmed Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited
title_sort languages support efficient communication about the environment: words for snow revisited
publisher eScholarship, University of California
publishDate 2016
url http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d
op_coverage e0151138
genre eskimo*
genre_facet eskimo*
op_source Regier, T; Carstensen, A; & Kemp, C. (2016). Languages Support Efficient Communication about the Environment: Words for Snow Revisited. PLoS ONE, 11(4), e0151138. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0151138. UC Berkeley: Retrieved from: http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d
op_relation qt2qb5722d
http://www.escholarship.org/uc/item/2qb5722d
op_rights public
op_doi https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151138
container_title PLOS ONE
container_volume 11
container_issue 4
container_start_page e0151138
_version_ 1766403081261547520