Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience
Some Canada Geese (Branta canadensis) raise their broods by themselves (two-parent families), while others raise them in gang broods, defined as two or more broods amalgamated into a single cohesive unit and shepherded by four or more parents. From 1984 to 2005, I individually marked Canada Geese in...
Published in: | The Condor |
---|---|
Main Author: | |
Format: | Text |
Language: | English |
Published: |
American Ornithological Society
2009
|
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 |
id |
ftbioone:10.1525/cond.2009.080073 |
---|---|
record_format |
openpolar |
spelling |
ftbioone:10.1525/cond.2009.080073 2024-05-12T08:02:01+00:00 Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience Michael R. Conover Michael R. Conover world 2009-05-01 text/HTML https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 en eng American Ornithological Society doi:10.1525/cond.2009.080073 All rights reserved. https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 Text 2009 ftbioone https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 2024-04-16T02:14:21Z Some Canada Geese (Branta canadensis) raise their broods by themselves (two-parent families), while others raise them in gang broods, defined as two or more broods amalgamated into a single cohesive unit and shepherded by four or more parents. From 1984 to 2005, I individually marked Canada Geese in New Haven County, Connecticut, so that I could compare the characteristics of adults that raise their goslings in gang broods to those of adults that raised their goslings in two-parent families. I wanted to determine if a parent's decision to form a gang brood was influenced by its age or body mass, its own parents (indicating either that the behavior has a genetic component or that the behavior is learned while a gosling), its prior experiences raising broods, or the loss of its mate. Parents tended to use the same brood-rearing approach from one year to the next: 61% of parents of gang broods (i.e., gang-brooders) during one year also were gang-brooders the next year they had goslings; likewise, 65% of parents in two-parent families during one year raised their next brood in a two-parent family. Geese that changed mates from the previous year were more likely to switch brood-rearing approaches than those that stayed with the same mate. As geese gained more years of experience raising goslings, their propensity to form a gang brood increased; only 29% of geese raising broods for the first time formed a gang brood versus 80% for geese with 5 or more years of experience. Geese raised in gang broods themselves were no more likely than geese raised in two-parent families to form gang broods once they became adults and had their own broods. These results indicate that gang brooding is a behavior learned as an adult. I tested the hypothesis that adult geese attending the same gang brood are members of the same extended family but found that geese were as likely to form a gang brood with unrelated individuals as with siblings or parents. Text Branta canadensis BioOne Online Journals Canada The Condor 111 2 276 282 |
institution |
Open Polar |
collection |
BioOne Online Journals |
op_collection_id |
ftbioone |
language |
English |
description |
Some Canada Geese (Branta canadensis) raise their broods by themselves (two-parent families), while others raise them in gang broods, defined as two or more broods amalgamated into a single cohesive unit and shepherded by four or more parents. From 1984 to 2005, I individually marked Canada Geese in New Haven County, Connecticut, so that I could compare the characteristics of adults that raise their goslings in gang broods to those of adults that raised their goslings in two-parent families. I wanted to determine if a parent's decision to form a gang brood was influenced by its age or body mass, its own parents (indicating either that the behavior has a genetic component or that the behavior is learned while a gosling), its prior experiences raising broods, or the loss of its mate. Parents tended to use the same brood-rearing approach from one year to the next: 61% of parents of gang broods (i.e., gang-brooders) during one year also were gang-brooders the next year they had goslings; likewise, 65% of parents in two-parent families during one year raised their next brood in a two-parent family. Geese that changed mates from the previous year were more likely to switch brood-rearing approaches than those that stayed with the same mate. As geese gained more years of experience raising goslings, their propensity to form a gang brood increased; only 29% of geese raising broods for the first time formed a gang brood versus 80% for geese with 5 or more years of experience. Geese raised in gang broods themselves were no more likely than geese raised in two-parent families to form gang broods once they became adults and had their own broods. These results indicate that gang brooding is a behavior learned as an adult. I tested the hypothesis that adult geese attending the same gang brood are members of the same extended family but found that geese were as likely to form a gang brood with unrelated individuals as with siblings or parents. |
author2 |
Michael R. Conover |
format |
Text |
author |
Michael R. Conover |
spellingShingle |
Michael R. Conover Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience |
author_facet |
Michael R. Conover |
author_sort |
Michael R. Conover |
title |
Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience |
title_short |
Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience |
title_full |
Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience |
title_fullStr |
Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience |
title_full_unstemmed |
Gang Brooding in Canada Geese: Role of Parental Condition and Experience |
title_sort |
gang brooding in canada geese: role of parental condition and experience |
publisher |
American Ornithological Society |
publishDate |
2009 |
url |
https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 |
op_coverage |
world |
geographic |
Canada |
geographic_facet |
Canada |
genre |
Branta canadensis |
genre_facet |
Branta canadensis |
op_source |
https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 |
op_relation |
doi:10.1525/cond.2009.080073 |
op_rights |
All rights reserved. |
op_doi |
https://doi.org/10.1525/cond.2009.080073 |
container_title |
The Condor |
container_volume |
111 |
container_issue |
2 |
container_start_page |
276 |
op_container_end_page |
282 |
_version_ |
1798844119691296768 |