Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?

Abstract and Summary How a Common Tern ( Sterna hirundo ) Colony Defends itself against Herring Gulls ( Larus argentatus ) The subject of this study is the anti‐predator behaviour of a small common tern colony near a large herring gull colony on the island of Mellum, West Germany (Fig. 1). In 1980 t...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Published in:Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie
Main Author: Becker, Peter H.
Format: Article in Journal/Newspaper
Language:English
Published: Wiley 1984
Subjects:
Online Access:http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
https://api.wiley.com/onlinelibrary/tdm/v1/articles/10.1111%2Fj.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
id crwiley:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
record_format openpolar
spelling crwiley:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x 2024-06-02T08:05:28+00:00 Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein? Becker, Peter H. 1984 http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x https://api.wiley.com/onlinelibrary/tdm/v1/articles/10.1111%2Fj.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x en eng Wiley http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie volume 66, issue 4, page 265-288 ISSN 0044-3573 journal-article 1984 crwiley https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x 2024-05-03T11:00:54Z Abstract and Summary How a Common Tern ( Sterna hirundo ) Colony Defends itself against Herring Gulls ( Larus argentatus ) The subject of this study is the anti‐predator behaviour of a small common tern colony near a large herring gull colony on the island of Mellum, West Germany (Fig. 1). In 1980 the number of gulls crossing this tern colony increased during the terns' chick‐stage (Fig. 4). Observed predation of tern chicks was independent of tide and time of day (3., Fig. 5). The frequency of tern reactions corresponded to the number of herring gull crossings (Fig. 5, Table 1). The terns' responses increased between morning and evening (Fig. 8). Tern up‐flights and attacks increased absolutely and as a percentage, with the advance of the breeding season (Fig. 3, 4). They were positively correlated with the observed chick predation and the number of pairs with chicks, most markedly with chicks older than 5 days (Figs. 3, 4; Table 1). This increased defence was maintained by fewer pairs as, by then, many had lost their own broods (Fig. 4). As the breeding season progressed, herring gulls increasingly became the main cause of tern up‐flights and the object of the attacks (Figs. 9–11). The up‐flights of the whole colony, which occurred frequently and spontaneously during incubation, were observed only rarely after hatching and were almost exclusively a response to herring gulls (Figs. 10, 12). The lower herring gulls flew over the colony, the more frequently common terns flew up or attacked and the more individuals were involved in these responses (Figs. 6, 13, 14). During the breeding period, communal up‐flights and attacks by terns increased as a percentage (Figs. 12, 13, 15–17). Group‐attacks effected changes in the gulls' flying‐routes more often than did individual attacks (Fig. 18). Despite the defence behaviour and its adaptation to the predation pressure, herring gulls often succeeded in robbing chicks. This is why the breeding success of the common tern was poor (< 0.4 chicks/nest). Possible reasons ... Article in Journal/Newspaper Common tern Sterna hirundo Wiley Online Library Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 66 4 265 288
institution Open Polar
collection Wiley Online Library
op_collection_id crwiley
language English
description Abstract and Summary How a Common Tern ( Sterna hirundo ) Colony Defends itself against Herring Gulls ( Larus argentatus ) The subject of this study is the anti‐predator behaviour of a small common tern colony near a large herring gull colony on the island of Mellum, West Germany (Fig. 1). In 1980 the number of gulls crossing this tern colony increased during the terns' chick‐stage (Fig. 4). Observed predation of tern chicks was independent of tide and time of day (3., Fig. 5). The frequency of tern reactions corresponded to the number of herring gull crossings (Fig. 5, Table 1). The terns' responses increased between morning and evening (Fig. 8). Tern up‐flights and attacks increased absolutely and as a percentage, with the advance of the breeding season (Fig. 3, 4). They were positively correlated with the observed chick predation and the number of pairs with chicks, most markedly with chicks older than 5 days (Figs. 3, 4; Table 1). This increased defence was maintained by fewer pairs as, by then, many had lost their own broods (Fig. 4). As the breeding season progressed, herring gulls increasingly became the main cause of tern up‐flights and the object of the attacks (Figs. 9–11). The up‐flights of the whole colony, which occurred frequently and spontaneously during incubation, were observed only rarely after hatching and were almost exclusively a response to herring gulls (Figs. 10, 12). The lower herring gulls flew over the colony, the more frequently common terns flew up or attacked and the more individuals were involved in these responses (Figs. 6, 13, 14). During the breeding period, communal up‐flights and attacks by terns increased as a percentage (Figs. 12, 13, 15–17). Group‐attacks effected changes in the gulls' flying‐routes more often than did individual attacks (Fig. 18). Despite the defence behaviour and its adaptation to the predation pressure, herring gulls often succeeded in robbing chicks. This is why the breeding success of the common tern was poor (< 0.4 chicks/nest). Possible reasons ...
format Article in Journal/Newspaper
author Becker, Peter H.
spellingShingle Becker, Peter H.
Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?
author_facet Becker, Peter H.
author_sort Becker, Peter H.
title Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?
title_short Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?
title_full Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?
title_fullStr Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?
title_full_unstemmed Wie richtet eine Flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( Sterna hirundo) ihr Abwehrverhalten auf den Feinddruck durch Silbermöwen ( Larus argentatus) ein?
title_sort wie richtet eine flußseeschwalbenkolonie ( sterna hirundo) ihr abwehrverhalten auf den feinddruck durch silbermöwen ( larus argentatus) ein?
publisher Wiley
publishDate 1984
url http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
https://api.wiley.com/onlinelibrary/tdm/v1/articles/10.1111%2Fj.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/pdf/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
genre Common tern
Sterna hirundo
genre_facet Common tern
Sterna hirundo
op_source Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie
volume 66, issue 4, page 265-288
ISSN 0044-3573
op_rights http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/termsAndConditions#vor
op_doi https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1984.tb01369.x
container_title Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie
container_volume 66
container_issue 4
container_start_page 265
op_container_end_page 288
_version_ 1800750271301681152